Sunday, June 2, 2019
Operant and Classical Conditioning in Advertising
operative and Classical Conditioning in AdvertisingBuyer deportment and marketing communicationsClassical Conditioning TheoryOperant breeding hypothesis skunk be compared to virtuous learn learning theory, as some(prenominal) are methods through which behaviour can be modified. The make difference is that operative learning is based on modifying the consequences of behaviour, whilst unpolluted instruct is based on the use of stimuli to modify behaviour. This can most clearly be seen by reference to Pavlovs (1927) and Skinners (1957) experiments. Pavlovs (1927) experiments knobbed training dogs to associate the sound of a bell with being fed. As such, the dogs learnt to drool when they heard a bell, in anticipation of food, in spite of not controlling when the food was delivered. This demonstrates upright learn learning. In contrast, Skinners (1957) experiments involved placing rats in separate boxes each of which contained a bar. When the rat pressed the bar, a consequ ence would occur. Some rats were fed when they pressed their bar, whilst others suffered an electric car shock. Other rats were given an electric shock until they pressed the bar, when the shock went away. Over time, the rats learnt to moderate their behaviour, either to obtain the benefits or to avoid the harmful mental pictures. This is an example of operant learning (Domjan, 2003).As a result of the interactive nature of operant learning, advertising has traditionally been based on classical condition, with campaigns generally intended to drive dynamic learning within consumers, thus moderating their behaviour to associate a product with a certain type of reward. Ackerberg (2003) claims that this reward can be either some make water of tangible benefits or it can be based on image, check off or prestige benefits. However, Ackerbergs (2003) look indicated that traditional advertising does not always tend to affect consumer behaviour, instead simply acting to inform consumers and enable them to choose products which fit their self image. This is beef upd by Osselaer and Alba (2000) who argue that employ the advertising process to attempt to enhance brand equity can often result in a decrease in customer knowledge of the attributes utilise to judge product quality. This is particularly the case when a company already has an existing reputation based on product quality, indicating that consumers are more than likely to trust the operant instruct they prepare developed from using products, rather than the perception of verbalise products.However, the recent rise of interactive media such as e-mail and the internet offer companies the chance to include more operant condition principles in the advertising and promotional campaigns, by allowing customers to interact with products through practical(prenominal) multimedia environments. This efficaciously allows companies to achieve a balance between indirect advertising and direct, but expensive and n arrowly focuse, product trials, whilst maintaining a wide reach. Indeed, research by Daugherty et al (2008) has shown that when comparing the direct, indirect, and virtual marketing methods, the virtual methodology can have the greatest impact on brand attitudes, particularly when it is followed up by exposure to advertising or a direct trial. This indicates that virtual advertising allows companies to obtain the best of both(prenominal) worlds, aiding in the indirect, classical conditioning, approach and the direct, operant learning, approach (Daugherty et al, 2008). As such, the consequence tends to suggest that both operant and classical learning can be used in the creation of advertising campaigns.However, evidence from Pawlowski et al (1998) shows that existing conditioning and learning pull up stakes as well play an important role in perceptions of advertising, and the ability of campaigns to affect consumer behaviour. This was demonstrated by Pawlowski et als experiments with children in the American second, fourth, and sixth grades, who were shown four advertisements. The older children were better able to interpret the metaphors from the advertisements, as well as the brand names and proper(postnominal) advertising copy. However, in spite of the increased levels of understanding with age, there was little evidence that the advertising messages improved the perceptions of the advertised products. This not only indicates that peoples experience and development lead affect their interpretation of advertising, but also that it is important to ensure that advertising is correctly pitched in order to reinforce or punish existing perceptions to boost the operant conditioning effect.Some evidences as to how this can be achieved can be found in the imprint of Ottemann and Lethans (1975), who argued that operant learning theory and behavioural modification principles could be used to affect the organisational behaviour of major organisations. Their work was based on the use of organisational behaviour modification programs, and their effectuate on the performance of first line supervisors who had been ingenious to manage their behaviour on a contingent basis. The results of said study revealed that supervisors who had been trained to moderate their behaviour, according to the operant conditions, achieved consistently superior performance levels when compared to their peers who had experienced no such training (Ottemann and Luthans, 1975). This could be argued to indicate that operant learning theory is always of use in moderating behaviour, and that this usefulness tends to be increased as the relevancy of the stimulus in increased.Within advertising, till and Priluck (2000) refer to the relevance of a stimulus, and also to stimulus generalisation, which refers to the extent to which a response conditioned to one stimulus transfers to similar stimuli. Indeed, in the context of using conditioning and learning for advertising camp aigns, stimulus generalisation is vital for factors such as brand extension and brand imitation. This argument is supported by deuce studies carried out by Till and Priluck (2000), which show that once an advertising or promotion campaign has conditioned customer attitudes towards a particular brand, they can then transfer said attitudes to a product in a similar category, or with a similar name. However, the results of the studies also suggested that this may only be a temporary effect, and that customers leave alone ultimately also need to be conditioned to the new product or category (Till and Priluck, 2000). In addition, Kim et al (1998) argue that existing attitudes and the nature of an operant learning approach can affect the extent to which these attitudes can be transferred, as well as the formation of new attitudes and product beliefs. This continues to indicate that both operant learning and classical conditioning will run into problems if they attempt to run against exis ting environmental conditioning which exists in consumers.However, Grossman and Till (1998) carried out two experiments which tended to indicate that classical conditioning learning can be used to overcome existing environmental conditioning through advertising, although this was found to be a long term effect. This finding is based on the use of classical conditioning procedures which matched favourable images and representations to a brand of mouthwash. The first experiment was based on simple exposure, the impact of which was then measured immediately after the experiment and again after one week and three weeks had elapsed. The second study used the same methods and procedures, but in a more controlled environment and over a longer period of time. This showed that not only do classical conditioning adverts tend to be durable in the minds of consumers, but also that they can gradually alter existing perceptions over time (Grossman and Till, 1998). Similar results were shown by H err and Fazio (1991), withal their research also examined the use of both attributional and cognitive, verbal learning techniques in order to maximise the conditioning affects. Such repeated and varied positioning of the brand as being favourably evaluated was shown to be more effective than simply using a single technique (Herr and Fazio, 1991).Another area in which both classical and operant learning and conditioning can be used in advertising is in the use of celebrities as conditioned stimuli to help with the associative learning process. Recent work by Till et al (2008) has established that the use of celebrities in advertising can actually have an active, operant conditioning, effect on conditioning, in spite of advertising being more of a classical conditioning medium. In addition to this, the operant learning effects will tend to be more effective when there is a fit between celebrity and product, and will also tend to be more enduring than peer products with no celebrity e ndorsement (Till et al, 2008). Similar observations have been made for the use of event or sports team sponsorship, although this has been shown to have more of a classical conditioning than an operant learning effect (Speed and Thompson, 2000). Again, the effectiveness of this approach is driven by the fit between the sponsor and event or team as well as perceptions of the sponsor and their motivations for the sponsorship (Speed and Thompson, 2000).However, in spite of the now widespread use of classical conditioning as a method for interpreting the effects of advertising, and the less wide spread use of operant learning, there are still critics of these approaches. In particular, Allen and Madden (1985) argue that there is a need for a more precise affective-conditioning hypothesis to be created in order to better describe the effects of advertising, and in the absence of unequivocal evidence to support classical conditioning effects. In addition, Stuart et al (1987) conducted exp eriments to determine exactly what classical conditioning effects could be observed within the context of advertising and consumer behaviour. These experiments demonstrated that whilst classical attitude conditioning could be observed at all four levels of stimulus pairing, environmental conditioning and latent inhibition acted to strongly retard these conditioning effects. This implies that the affect of classical conditioning approaches in advertising cannot be accurately predicted, due to the wide range of environmental and latent inhibitions which will be inborn in the majority of consumers who have already been exposed to advertising for any great length of time (Stuart et al, 1987).One example of an advertising campaign which has used classical conditioning and operant learning is the move by the supermarket chain Sainsburys to use celebrity chef Jamie Oliver to front a major advertising campaign, and effectively be the TV face of Sainsburys (Wheeler, 2003). This advertising campaign can be seen to have a strong classical conditioning effect, as it helps customers to associate the products and mark that they see in Sainsburys stores with a well recognised culinary expert. In addition, the use of a relevant celebrity will provide operant learning, something which has been supported by the inclusion of promotional materials such as Jamie Oliver recipe cards, which have been made available to customers. Wheeler (2003) provides evidence of the success of the operant conditioning impact of this campaign after Oliver demonstrated how to use Sainsburys truffle butter to cook an appetising Christmas turkey, Sainsburys reported selling more than 50,000 jars of the product. This shows that Olivers recommendations, shown on TV, on recipe cards and in store, conditioned customers to associate the butter with cooking a high quality meal, thus increasing sales.However, there is also evidence that Sainsburys has suffered somewhat from existing environmental condition ing towards Oliver, with Walker (2005) reporting that Oliver is someone that people either love or hate. As such, some people will already have negative conditioning nearly Oliver, and this conditioning will act to inhibit any conditioning around Sainsburys brand and products. In addition, Olivers actions, such as refusing to use Sainsburys farmed salmon in his restaurants despite endorsing it for the company, will also impair conditioning as people will perceive a conflict between the two stimuli (Walker, 2005). On addition, Olivers personal views can also condition customers against the Sainsburys brand, as reported by the Telegraph (2008) when Oliver attacked Sainsburys for failing to turn up to a debate on factory farming. Whilst the company allowed a superior director to be interviewed as part of the program, and none of the other major supermarkets sent representatives to the debate, the conditioned connexion between Oliver and Sainsburys means that the consequences of non attendance will likely be higher for Sainsburys, and may further impair conditioning.In addition, Sainsburys should look to boost its use of Oliver in more operant conditioning settings, in order to overcome these issues. For example, the supermarket could make use of the interactive nature of the internet to set up a cook with Jamie part of the site, where customers could download recipes and hitch how they are cooked. This would help them make associations between buying Sainsburys products and cooking appetising meals, as well as making it easier for them to cook meals once they have bought the ingredients. The campaign could also be boosted by the use of more in store activities associated with Oliver, such as holding in store tastings of products he has endorsed, or demonstrating their use in store. For example, in the week before Xmas, by holding a demonstration of how to use the truffle butter mentioned above to cook a turkey, Sainsburys could have benefitted from operant co nditioning and learning, and further boosted its sales of the product.In conclusion, in spite of the difficulties associated with using both operant conditioning and classical conditioning in advertising and promotions, it is clear that both techniques still have significant potential applications to real life advertising campaigns. In particular, the use of the internet and association with well known celebrities allows retailers, and other businesses, to benefits from the combined effects of classical conditioning and operant learning, to further boost associations. By doing so, and ensuring that the associations are relevant and pertinent to the customers, retailers can achieve both short term success, as seen in the truffle butter example above, and long term success, as described by Grossman and Till (1998), by making long term adjustments to the environmental conditioning of their customers.ReferencesAckerberg, D. A. (2003) Advertising, learning, and consumer choice in experie nce good markets an empirical examination. International Economic Review Vol. 44, outgrowth 3, p. 1007-1040.Daugherty, T. Li, H. and Biocca, F. (2008) Consumer learning and the effects of virtual experience relative to indirect and direct product experience. Psychology Marketing Vol. 25, edit 7, p. 568-586.Domjan, M. E. (2003) The Principles of schooling and Behaviour Fifth Edition. Belmont, CA Thomson / Wadsworth.Grossman, R. P. and Till, B. D. (1998) The Persistence of Classically Conditioned Brand Attitudes. Journal of Advertising Vol. 27, Issue 1, p. 23-31.Herr, P. M. and Fazio, R. H. (1991) On the Effectiveness of Repeated Positive Expressions as an Advertising Strategy. Advances in Consumer interrogation Vol. 18, Issue 1, p. 30-32.Kim, J. Jeen-Su L. and Bhargava, M. (1998) The Role of Affect in Attitude Formation A Classical Conditioning Approach. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science Vol. 26, Issue 2, p. 143-152.Osselaer, S. M. J. A. and Alba, J. W. (2000) Consumer Learning and Brand Equity. Journal of Consumer Research Vol. 27, Issue 1, p. 1-16.Ottemann, R. and Luthans, F. (1975) An Experimental Analysis Of The Effectiveness Of An Organizational Behavior Modification Program In Industry. Academy of Management legal proceeding p. 140-142.Pavlov, I. P. (1927) Conditioned Reflexes An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex. London Oxford University Press.Pawlowski, D. R. Badzinski, D. M. and Mitchell, N. (1998) Effects of Metaphors on Childrens Comprehension and Perception of Print Advertisements. Journal of Advertising Vol. 27, Issue 2, p. 83-98.Skinner, B. F. (1957) communicative Learning. New York Appleton-Century-Crofts.Speed, R. and Thompson, P. (2000) Determinants of Sports Sponsorship Response. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science Vol. 28, Issue 2, p. 227-238.Stuart, E. W. Shimp, T. A. and Engle, R. W. (1987) Classical Conditioning of Consumer Attitudes Four Experiments in an Advertising Context. Journ al of Consumer Research Vol. 14, Issue 3, p. 334-349.Telegraph, The (2008) Sainsburys acting chicken, says Jamie Oliver. The Daily Telegraph and Telegraph.co.uk. 8th January 2008. Accessed 20th July 2008 http//www.telegraph.co.uk/news/uknews/1574897/Sainsburys-playing-chicken,-says-Jamie-Oliver.htmlTill, B. D. and Priluck, R. L. (2000) Stimulus Generalization in Classical Conditioning An Initial Investigation and Extension. Psychology Marketing Vol. 17, Issue 1, p. 55-72.Till, B. D. Stanley, S. M. and Priluck, R. L. (2008) Classical conditioning and celebrity endorsers An examination of belongingness and resistance to extinction. Psychology Marketing Vol. 25, Issue 2, p. 179-196.Walker, A. (2005) Profile Jamie Oliver. BBC News Magazine. 30th March 2005. Accessed 20th July 2008 http//news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/magazine/4394025.stmWheeler, B. (2003) Sainsbury banks on fresh Oliver ads. BBC News Business. 11th June 2003. Accessed 20th July 2008 http//news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/business/2979646.st m
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.